I am not this hair or this skin, I'm the soul that lives within...

masyhead

The Cur­rent Year is 6265


Humans Are Being Taught to Echolo­cate Like Dol­phins — and It’s Sur­pris­ingly Easy

Sci­en­tists have suc­cess­fully taught a small group of blind and sighted peo­ple how to nav­i­gate their sur­round­ings using echolo­ca­tion — the sonar-​based lan­guage of dol­phins and bats.
Using sound cre­ated by tongue clicks, the group learned how to detect the size of vir­tual rooms with sur­pris­ing accu­racy — some­thing that researchers had not expected in peo­ple who were born with sight.
While blind peo­ple have proven suc­cess­ful at echolo­ca­tion in the past, it’s been unclear if sighted peo­ple can develop the same abil­ity, given their almost total depen­dence on visual perception.

We thought, ‘If it’s sighted peo­ple, it’s not going to be some­thing we’ve ever learned to do, so prob­a­bly we’re really bad at it,’” one of the team, Vir­ginia Flana­gin from the Lud­wig Max­i­m­il­ian Uni­ver­sity of Munich, Ger­many, told Veronique Green­wood at The Atlantic.
But the results showed the oppo­site — in an exper­i­ment involv­ing 11 sighted peo­ple and one blind per­son, the best-​performing sighted per­son could use echolo­ca­tion to detect a mere 4 per­cent dif­fer­ence in the size of a vir­tual room.
“Even the peo­ple who did less well could still often tell apart dif­fer­ences of 6 to 8 per­cent, with the least skilled bot­tom­ing out at a 16 per­cent dif­fer­ence,” Green­wood reports.
“Over­all, that actu­ally is about the same level of acu­ity — abil­ity to dis­tin­guish dif­fer­ences — that you find in some visual tests, says Flana­gin.”
To fig­ure this out, the team first trained their sub­jects in echolo­ca­tion by plac­ing them in a heav­ily padded ane­choic cham­ber, and play­ing record­ings of clicks made pre­vi­ously in real-​life build­ings.
By run­ning the exer­cise in a room that pro­duces no echoes of its own, the researchers could tell the par­tic­i­pants which sounds cor­re­lated to larger or smaller rooms, giv­ing them the oppor­tu­nity to learn the sub­tle dif­fer­ences between the two.
Once the vol­un­teers had got­ten through the ini­tial train­ing, the team hooked them into an MRI machine, which was con­nected to a vir­tual, 3D model of a nearby chapel build­ing.
The vol­un­teers would either click their tongue to make a sound, or the machine would make the sound for them — referred to as “active” and “pas­sive” echolo­ca­tion — and lis­ten for how those sounds echoed through the vir­tual room.
Based on these echoes, the vol­un­teers would have to judge the size of the vir­tual room.
The researchers found that the vol­un­teers all did sig­nif­i­cantly bet­ter when they were per­form­ing active echolo­ca­tion — mean­ing their own clicks were a far more effec­tive tool for them to nav­i­gate their vir­tual sur­round­ings.
That makes sense, see­ing as the vol­un­teers were more actively engaged in the exha­la­tion when they were the ones per­form­ing it, but what the researchers found strange was that the sound of the echoes acti­vated the sighted vol­un­teers’ motor cor­tex — the region of the brain respon­si­ble for move­ment.
Even when the team com­pared MRI scans from active and pas­sive echolo­ca­tion — allow­ing them to iso­late and remove the brain activ­ity involved in actu­ally mak­ing the phys­i­cal click­ing sound — that part of the brain still showed signs of life.
In fact, the motor cor­tex was found to be most active with large ver­sions of the chapel than smaller ones, Green­wood reports, which sug­gests a con­nec­tion between vir­tu­ally nav­i­gat­ing and phys­i­cally nav­i­gat­ing a space.
“It seems like the motor cor­tex is some­how involved in the sen­sory pro­cess­ing,” Flana­gin told her.
In the blind sub­ject, the echoes acti­vated the unused visual cor­tex instead, sug­gest­ing that they were visu­al­is­ing the echoes as they bounced between the vir­tual walls.
We should note that the study is extremely small, with a lim­ited sam­ple size of both blind and sighted peo­ple, so we can’t read too much into the results until they have been repli­cated in a much larger, more diverse group.
But based on what we know about human echolo­ca­tion already, it sug­gests that sighted peo­ple do have the capac­ity for this purely sound-​based form of nav­i­ga­tion.
You can watch one of the most famous human echolo­ca­tion experts, Daniel Kish, demon­strate his abil­ity rid­ing a bike in the video below:
The research has been pub­lished in the Jour­nal of Neu­ro­science.


Whats New in Gaza

If you do nothing else, Check out the forgotten post section on the home page (Top Left). It's the reason the site was made. If you are going to court read listen to Free Speech Radio, Seat-belts 1 & 2 and Cog­ni­tive Dis­so­nance at High Fre­quency! In that order. Posted in Audio and text format.

Why You Should Not Wear A Mask!

Who Dat ?

We have 70 guests and no mem­bers online